Mechanism-general and mechanism-specific explanations of Whorfian effects

Authors

  • Nicolás Alejandro Serrano CONICET, Universidad de Buenos Aires

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.5007/1808-1711.2022.e80272

Keywords:

Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, linguistic relativity, grounded approach, perception, perceptual experience, color

Abstract

Recent empirical evidence has revitalized the interested in the so-called "Whorfean effects" in perception, their reach, and underlying mechanisms. In this paper I propose that explanations of these effects fall into two different types: authors like Lupyan (2012) postulate mechanisms that are specifically designed to produce whorfean effects, while authors like Casasanto (2008) explain those effects as subproducts of more general mechanisms, such as those responsible of associative learning. Then, I show how the grounded approach about concepts in cognitive science (e.g. Barsalou 1999, 2016) could be relevant for the study of whorfean effects in linguistics, phylosophy and cognitive psychology. In particular, I'll hold that the evidence in favor of the grounded approach also favours "general mechanisms" explanations such as Casasanto's over "specific mechanism" explanations such as Lupyan's.

References

Anderson, M.L. 2010. Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain, Behav. Brain Sci. 33: 245–266, discussion 266–313.

Au, T. 1983. Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis revisited, Cognition 15: 155–187

Barsalou, L.W. 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22: 577–609; discussion 610–660.

Barsalou, L.W. 2012. The human conceptual system. In: M. Spivey; M. Joanisse; K. Mcrae (ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics, p.239–258. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Barsalou, L.W. 2016. On Staying Grounded and Avoiding Quixotic Dead Ends. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23: 1122–1142.

Barsalou, L.; Simmons, W.; Barbey, A.; Wilson, C. 2003. Grounding Conceptual Knowledge in Modality-Specific Systems. Trends in cognitive sciences 7: 84–91. 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3.

Beauchamp, M.S.; Haxby, J.V.; Jennings, J.E.; DeYoe, E.A. 1999. An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-Hue test reveals multiple color-selective areas in human ventral occipitotemporal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 9: 257–263.

Whorf, B.L. 1947. Science and Linguistics. In: T. H. Newcomb & E. L. Hartley (eds.), Readings in Social Psychology. New York: Henry Holt.

Whorf, B.L. 1956. Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Ed. by J.B. Carroll. New York: Technology Press of MIT.

Bird, C.M.; Berens, S.C.; Horner, A.J.; Franklin, A. 2014. Categorical encoding of color in the brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111: 4590–4595.

Björk, I. 2008. Relativizing Linguistic Relativity: Investigating Underlying Assumptions about Language in the Neo-Whorfian Literature. Uppsala: Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis.

Blomberg, J. & Zlatev, J. 2009. Linguistic relativity, mediation and thecategorization of motion. In: J. Zlatev; M. Andrén; M. Johansson Falck; C. Lundmark (eds.), Studies in Language and Cognition, p.46–61. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Bloom, A. 1981. The linguistic shaping of thought: A study in the impact of language on thinking in China and the West. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bloom, P. & Keil, F. C. 2001. Thinking through language. Mind Lang. 16: 351–367.

Boeckx, C. 2014. The Roots of Current Biolinguistic Thought: Revisiting the “Chomsky-Piaget Debate” in the Context of the Revival of Biolinguistics. Teorema: Revista Internacional de Filosofía 33(1): 83–94.

Cappe, C. & Barone, P. 2005. Heteromodal connections supporting multisensory integration at low levels of cortical processing in the monkey. Eur J Neurosci 22: 2886–2902.

Casasanto, D. 2008. Who’s Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought. Language Learning 58: 63-79.

Catani, M. & Ffytche, D.H. 2005. The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. Brain 128: 2224–2239.

Clavagnier, S.; Falchier, A.; Kennedy, H. 2004. Long-distance feedback projections to area V1: implications for multisensory integration, spatial awareness, and visual consciousness. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4: 117–126.

Damasio, A.R. 1989. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 33(1-2): 25-62.

Davidson, D. 1974. On the Very Idea of a Conceptual Scheme. Proceedings and Ad-dresses of the American Philosophical Association 47: 5–20.

Dennett, D.C. 1991. Consciousness Explained. Toronto, ON: Little Brown.

Fairhall, S.L. & Caramazza, A. 2013. Brain regions that represent amodal conceptual knowledge. J Neurosci 33(25): 10552-8.

Falchier, A.; Clavagnier, S.; Barone, P.; Kennedy, H. 2002. Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. J Neurosci 22: 5749–5759.

Falchier, A.; Schroeder, C.E.; Hackett, T.A.; Lakatos, P.; Nascimento-Silva, S.; Ulbert, I.; Karmos, G.; Smiley, J.F. 2010. Projection from visual areas V2 and prostriata to caudal auditory cortex in the monkey. Cereb Cortex 20: 1529–1538.

Feuer, L.S. 1953. Sociological Aspects of the Relation between Language and Philosophy. Philosophy of Science 20(l): 85-100.

Firestone, C. & Scholl J.B. 2015. Cognition Does Not Affect Perception: Evaluating the Evidence for “Top-Down” Effects. The Behavioral and brain sciences 39: E229.

Fodor, J. 1984. Observation reconsidered. Philosophy of Science 51: 23–43.

Fodor, J. 2008. LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. New York: Oxford University Press.

Forder, L.; Taylor O.; Mankin, H.; Scott, R.B.; Franklin, A. 2016. Colour Terms Affect Detection of Colour and Colour-Associated Objects Suppressed from Visual Awareness. PLoS ONE 11(3): e0152212.

Forder, L.; He, X.; Franklin, A. 2017. Colour categories are reflected in sensory stages of colour perception when stimulus issues are resolved. PLoS ONE 12: e0178097.

González-Perilli, F.; Rebollo, I.; Maiche, A.; Arévalo, A. 2017. Blues in Two Different Spanish-Speaking Populations. Front. Commun. 2: e18.

Gomes, G. 1998. The timing of conscious experience: a critical review and reinterpretation of Libet’s research. Conscious Cogn. 7(4): 559-95.

Hackett, T.A.; Smiley, J.F.; Ulbert, I.; Karmos, G.; Lakatos, P.; De la Mothe, L.A.; Schroeder, C.E. 2007. Sources of somatosensory input to the caudal belt areas of auditory cortex. Perception 36: 1419–1430.

Haimovici, S. 2018. The Modal-Amodal Distinction in the Debate on Conceptual Format. Philosophies 3(2): 7. doi:10.3390/philosophies3020007

Henderson, D. 1994. Conceptual Schemes after Davidson. In: G. Preyer; F. Siebelt; A. Ulfig (eds.), Language, Mind, and Epistemology: Essays on Donald Davidson’s Philosophy, p. 171–198. Dordrecht: Springer.

Hoijer, H. (Ed.). 1954. Language in culture: Conference on the interrelations of language and other aspects of culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Howard, R.J.; Ffytche, D.H.; Barnes, J.; McKeefry, D.; Ha, Y.;Woodruff, P.W.; Bullmore, E.T.; Simmons, A.; Williams, S.C.; David, A.S.; et al. 1998. The functional anatomy of imagining and perceiving colour. Neuroreport 9: 1019–1023.

Hyman, B.T.; Van Hoesen, G.W.; Damasio A. 1987 Alzheimer’s disease: glutamate depletion in the hippocampal perforant pathway zone. Ann Neurol. 22: 37–40.

Jones, E.G. & Powell, T.P. 1970. An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain 93: 793–820.

Kiefer, M. & Pulvermüller, F. 2012. Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex 48(7): 805-25.

Koerner, E.F.K. 1992. The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis: A Preliminary History and a Bibliographical Essay. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 2: 173-198.

Leshinskaya, A. & Caramazza, A. 2016. For a cognitive neuroscience of concepts: Moving beyond the grounding issue. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23: 991–1001.

Lupyan, G. 2012. Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in psychology 3: 54.

Lupyan, G. & Ward, E. J. 2013. Language can boost otherwise unseen objects into visual awareness. Proceedings of the Natural Academy of Sciences 110(35): 14196-14201.

Machery, E. 2016. The amodal brain and the offloading hypothesis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23: 1090–1095.

Mahon, B. Z. 2015. What is embodied about cognition? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 30: 420–429.

Man, K.; Kaplan, J.; Damasio, H.; Damasio A. 2013 Neural convergence and divergence in the mammalian cerebral cortex: from experimental neuroanatomy to functional neuroimaging. J Comp Neurol 521(18): 4097-111.

Martin, A.; Haxby, J.V.; Lalonde, F.M.; Wiggs, C.L.; Ungerleider, L.G. 1995. Discrete Cortical Regions Associated with Knowledge of Color and Knowledge of Action. Science 270: 102–105.

Martin, A. 2016. GRAPES—Grounding representations in action, perception, and emotion systems: How object properties and categories are represented in the human brain. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23: 979–990.

McWhorter, J.H. 2014. The Language Hoax. Why the World Looks the Same in Any Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Meyer, K. & Damasio, A. 2009. Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends Neurosci. 32(7): 376-82.

Meyer, K.; Kaplan, J.T.; Essex, R.; Webber, C.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A. 2010. Predicting visual stimuli on the basis of activity in auditory cortices. Nat Neurosci. 13: 1–26.

Meyer, K.; Kaplan, J.T.; Essex, R.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A. 2011. Seeing touch is correlated with content-specific activity in primary somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex 21: 2113–2121.

Murphey, D.K.; Yoshor, D.; Beauchamp, M.S. 2008. Perception matches selectivity in the human anterior color center. Curr. Biol. CB 18: 216–220.

Paulesu, E.; Harrison, J.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Watson, J.D.; Goldstein, L.; Heather, J.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Frith, C.D. 1995. The physiology of coloured hearing. A PET activation study of colour-word synaesthesia. Brain J. Neurol. 118: 661–676.

Pandya, D. 1995. Anatomy of the auditory cortex. Rev Neurol. 151: 486–494.

Perry, L. K. & Lupyan, G. 2017. Clarifying the label–categorization link. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 32(8): 950–953.

Piatelli-Palmarini, M. 1994. Ever Since Language and Learning: Afterthoughts On the Piaget-Chomsky Debate. Cognition 50: 315-346.

Pinker, S. 2007 [1994]. The Language Instinct. New York: Harper Perennial Modern Classics.

Prinz, J. 2002. Furnishing the Mind: Concepts and their Perceptual Basis. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Pullum, G. K. 1991. The great Eskimo vocabulary hoax and other irreverent essays on the study of language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Pylyshyn, Z. 1980. Computation and Cognition: Issues in the Foundations of Cognitive Science. Behavorial and Brain Sciences 3: 111–132.

Rockland, K.S. & Pandya, D. 1979. Laminar origins and terminations of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 179: 3–20.

Rockland, K.S. & Ojima, H. 2003. Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. Int J Psychophysiol. 50: 19–26.

Schwiedrzik, C.M.; Bernstein, B.; Melloni, L. 2016. Motion along the mental number line reveals shared representations for numerosity and space. eLife 5: e10806.

Seltzer, B. & Pandya, D. 1976. Some Cortical Projections to the Parahippocampal Area in the Rhesus Monkey. Exp Neurol. 160: 146–160.

Seltzer, B. & Pandya, D. 1980. Converging visual and somatic sensory cortical input to the intraparietal sulcus of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res. 192: 339–351.

Simmons, W.K.; Ramjee, V.; Beauchamp, M.S.; McRae, K.; Martin, A.; Barsalou, L.W. 2007. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia 45: 2802–2810.

Speed L. J. & Majid, A. 2019. Grounding language in the neglected senses of touch, taste, and smell. Cognitive Neuropsychology. DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2019.1623188

Sun Y.; Cai Y.; Lu S. 2015. Hemispheric asymmetry in the influence of language on visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition 34: 16–27.

Thierry, G.; Athanasopoulos, P.; Wiggett, A.; Dering, B.; Kuipers, J.-R. 2009. Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 106: 4567–4570.

Thierry, G. 2016. Neurolinguistic Relativity: How Language Flexes Human Perception and Cognition. Language learning, 66(3): 690–713.

Van Hoesen, G.W. & Damasio, A. 1987. Neural correlates of cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. In: V.B. Mountcastle; F. Plum; S.R. Geiger (eds.), Handbook of Physiology, p.871-898. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.

Van Hoesen, G.W.; Hyman, B.T.; Damasio, A. 1991. Entorhinal cortex pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Hippocampus 1: 1–8.

Van Hoesen, G.W.; Pandya, D.N.; Butters, N. 1972. Cortical afferents to the entorhinal cortex of the Rhesus monkey. Science 175: 1471–1473.

Van Hoesen, G.W. & Pandya, D. 1975a. Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey I temporal lobe afferents. Brain Res. 95: 1–24.

Van Hoesen, G.W. & Pandya, D. 1975b. Some connections of the entorhinal (area 28) and perirhinal (area 35) cortices of the rhesus monkey III efferent connections. Brain Res. 95: 39–59.

Vogt, B.A. & Pandya, D. 1978. Cortico-cortical connections of somatic sensory cortex (areas 3, 1 and 2) in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 177: 179–191.

Vygotsky, L.S. 1962. Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Waldhauser, G.T.; Braun, V.; Hanslmayr, S. 2016. Episodic Memory Retrieval Functionally Relies on Very Rapid Reactivation of Sensory Information. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 36: 251–260.

Winawer, J.; Witthoft, N.; Frank, M.C.; Wu, L.; Wade, A.R.; Boroditsky, L. 2007. Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104: 7780–7785

Whorf, B. 1956. Science and Linguistics. In: J.B. Carrol (ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writing of Benjamin Lee Whorf, p.265-280. New York: The MIT Press.

Xia, T.; Xu, G.; Mo, L. 2018. Bi-lateralized Whorfian effect in color perception: Evidence from Chinese Sign Language. Journal of Neurolinguistics 49: 189-201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.07.004

Zlatev, J. & Blomberg, J. 2015. Language may indeed influence thought. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1631.

Published

2022-12-13

Issue

Section

Articles